Schemata
Last modified: 22 January 2011
Catherine Emmott & Marc Alexander
[1] 1 Definition
[2]
Schemata are cognitive structures representing generic knowledge, i.e. structures which do not contain information about particular entities, instances or events, but rather about their general form. Readers use schemata to make sense of events and descriptions by providing default background information for comprehension, as it is rare and often unnecessary for texts to contain all the detail required for them to be fully understood. Usually, many or even most of the details are omitted, and readers’ schemata compensate for any gaps in the text. As schemata represent the knowledge base of individuals, they are often culturally and temporally specific, and are ordinarily discussed as collective stores of knowledge shared by prototypical members of a given or assumed community. The term was used in the 1930s in both psychology and literary theory, but entered wider currency in the 1970s in Artificial Intelligence research, later being re-incorporated into psychology and thence into linguistics, within the general area of cognitive science.
[3] 2 Explication
[4]
The terms used in this area have historically been highly variable and differ across disciplines. The term “schema” is often used as a superordinate label for a broad range of knowledge structures, including frames, scenarios, scripts and plans, as described below. “Schema” is also used as a synonym for “frame” (Minsky Minsky, Marvin (1975). “A Framework for Representing Knowledge.” P. H. Winston (ed). The Psychology of Computer Vision. New York: McGraw-Hill, 211–77.1975) to refer to mental representations of objects, settings or situations. A restaurant schema/frame, for example, would contain information about types of restaurants, what objects are to be found inside a restaurant, and so on. The term “scenario” is also sometimes used for situational knowledge (Sanford & Garrod Sanford, Anthony J. & Simon C. Garrod (1981). Understanding Written Language: Explorations in Comprehension Beyond the Sentence. Chichester: Wiley.1981). A “script” (Schank & Abelson Schank, Roger C. & Robert P. Abelson (1977). Scripts, Plans, Goals and Understanding. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.1977) is a temporally-ordered schema; it describes a reader’s knowledge of stereotypical goal-oriented event sequences “that define a well-known situation” (422), so that a restaurant script would contain knowledge of the actions and sequence of ordering food, paying bills, and so on. In addition to a sequence of events, most scripts have further “slots” to describe the “roles” (customers, waiters, chefs, etc.), “props” (menu, table, food, money, bill, etc.), “entry conditions” (customer is hungry, restaurant has food, etc.) and “results” (customer is no longer hungry, restaurant has less food, etc.) within the script. A “plan” (Schank & Abelson Schank, Roger C. & Robert P. Abelson (1977). Scripts, Plans, Goals and Understanding. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.1977) consists of knowledge about sets of actions needed to accomplish objectives and is used in non-stereotypical situations where there is no adequate script available.
[5]
Linguists, psychologists and narrative scholars employ schema theory to account for the interpretation of a text where the discourse itself does not provide all the information necessary for the discourse to be processed. Consider the following example: “John went to a restaurant for lunch. He ordered a salad, had a coffee and then went to the park for a walk.” This short text cannot describe all the actions, activities and situational information which a reader requires to comprehend it. Schemata and scripts supply the gaps in reader knowledge (that, for example, a restaurant is a place which serves food, that food once ordered is supplied, and that one must pay before leaving). The general notion of gap-filling has long been recognized in literary studies. Ingarden (Ingarden, Roman ([1931] 1973). The Literary Work of Art: An Investigation on the Borderlines of Ontology, Logic and Theory of Literature. Evanston: Northwestern UP.1931) refers to “spots of indeterminacy,” an idea later adopted by Iser (Iser, Wolfgang ([1976] 1978). The Act of Reading: A Theory of Aesthetic Response. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.1976), and Sternberg (Sternberg, Meir (1978). Expositional Modes and Temporal Ordering in Fiction. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins UP.1978, Sternberg, Meir (1985). The Poetics of Biblical Narrative: Ideological Literature and the Drama of Reading. Bloomington: Indiana UP.1985) discusses “expositional gaps.” Research in Artificial Intelligence on schemata adds a detailed explanation of how inferences are made by utilizing generic knowledge in processing specific parts of a text. As schemata are situational and socioculturally dependent, some readers may supply more information from their schemata than others.
[6]
Schemata are therefore essential for establishing the coherence of a text (→ Coherence). Furthermore, schemata are dynamic (Schank Schank, Roger C. (1982). Dynamic Memory: A Theory of Reminding and Learning. Cambridge: Cambridge UP.1982) to the extent that they accumulate details and are altered in the course of experience. If changing circumstances and new events contradict existing schemata or make them appear inadequate in a relatively minor way, they can be “tuned” (Rumelhart Rumelhart, David E. (1980). “Schemata: The Building Blocks of Cognition.” R. Spiro, B. Bruce & W. Brewer (eds). Theoretical Issues in Reading Comprehension. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum, 33–58.1980: 52) to accommodate new generalizations. The relationship between texts and schemata is two-way: while schemata tend to lay the ground rules for how a discourse will be interpreted, discourses themselves may prompt readers to “tune” existing schemata and create new ones (Rumelhart & Norman Rumelhart, David E. & Donald A. Norman (1978). “Accretion, Tuning and Restructuring: Three Modes of Learning.” J. W. Cotton & R. Klatzky (eds). Semantic Factors in Cognition. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum, 37–53.1978; Cook Cook, Guy (1994). Discourse and Literature: The Interplay of Form and Mind. Oxford: Oxford UP.1994: 182–84).
[7] 3 History of the Concept and its Study
[8]
Some schema researchers (e.g. Cook Cook, Guy (1994). Discourse and Literature: The Interplay of Form and Mind. Oxford: Oxford UP.1994; Semino Semino, Elena (1997). Language and World Creation in Poems and Other Texts. London: Longman.1997) trace the philosophical notion of schemata back to Immanuel Kant. Another antecedent is Gestalt theory in psychology (Wertheimer Wertheimer, Max ([1923] 1938). “Laws of Organization in Perceptual Forms.” W. D. Ellis (ed). A Source Book of Gestalt Psychology. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul, 71–88.1923, Wertheimer, Max ([1925] 1938). “Gestalt Theory.” W. D. Ellis (ed). A Source Book of Gestalt Psychology. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul, 1–11.1925; Köhler Köhler, Wolfgang (1930). Gestalt Psychology. London: Bell.1930; Koffka Koffka, Kurt (1935). Principles of Gestalt Psychology. London: Kegan Paul, Trench, Trubner.1935). Also in psychology, Bartlett (Bartlett, Frederick C. (1932). Remembering: A Study in Experimental and Social Psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge UP.1932) used the term (which he credits to the earlier work of the neurologist Sir Henry Head) to explain speakers’ unknowing alteration of folktale details during retellings, with such alterations being made in line with the speakers’ schemata. In literary theory in the 1930s, Ingarden (Ingarden, Roman ([1931] 1973). The Literary Work of Art: An Investigation on the Borderlines of Ontology, Logic and Theory of Literature. Evanston: Northwestern UP.1931) argued that there was a stratum of “schematized aspects” in the perception of literary works of art. After a lull of many years, schema theory re-emerged in the 1970s and 1980s, when schemata were refined within Artificial Intelligence as mental constructs of knowledge derived from an individual’s experience and learning (in this sense often called “frames,” e.g. Minsky Minsky, Marvin (1975). “A Framework for Representing Knowledge.” P. H. Winston (ed). The Psychology of Computer Vision. New York: McGraw-Hill, 211–77.1975). While scripts were first identified by Schank & Abelson (Schank, Roger C. & Robert P. Abelson (1977). Scripts, Plans, Goals and Understanding. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.1977), the focus of their work was mainly on computational aspects of comprehension. Bower et al. (Bower, Gordon, John B. Black & Terrence J. Turner (1979). “Scripts in Memory for Text.” Cognitive Psychology 11, 177–220.1979) then provided evidence within cognitive psychology that readers employed scripts during their processing of a discourse. Later, Schank (Schank, Roger C. (1982). Dynamic Memory: A Theory of Reminding and Learning. Cambridge: Cambridge UP.1982) employed scripts in more detail as dynamic tools for discourse processing, breaking scripts down into component parts (Memory Organization Packets, MOPs) which could be combined into larger structures when required.
[9]
In narrative studies, schema theory has been important not only for its role in explaining gap-filling in reading, as discussed above, but also in relation to a reader’s knowledge of the overall structure of stories, termed “story schemata” (e.g. Rumelhart Rumelhart, David E. (1980). “Schemata: The Building Blocks of Cognition.” R. Spiro, B. Bruce & W. Brewer (eds). Theoretical Issues in Reading Comprehension. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum, 33–58.1975; Mandler & Johnson Mandler, Jean M. & Nancy S. Johnson (1977). “Remembrance of Things Parsed: Story Structure and Recall.” Cognitive Psychology 9, 111–51.1977; Mandler Mandler, Jean M. (1984). Scripts, Stories and Scenes: Aspects of Schema Theory. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.1984), the cognitive equivalent of text-based story grammars. According to their proponents, story schemata contain sets of expectations about how stories will continue, although some psychologists (e.g. Black & Wilensky Black, John B. & Robert Wilensky (1979). “An Evaluation of Story Grammars.” Cognitive Science 3, 213–30.1979; Johnson-Laird Johnson-Laird, Philip (1983). Mental Models. Cambridge: Cambridge UP.1983) have questioned whether special cognitive structures are required beyond general reasoning. Knowledge of the form of texts has also been studied in the analysis of “super-coherence,” de Beaugrande’s (de Beaugrande, Robert (1987). “Schemas for Literary Communication.” L. Halász (ed). Literary Discourse: Aspects of Cognitive and Social Psychological Approaches. Berlin: de Gruyter, 49–99.1987) term for thematic awareness, in postulating schemata for specific genres (Fludernik Fludernik, Monika (1996). Towards a ‘Natural’ Narratology. London: Routledge.1996; Herman Herman, David (2002). Story Logic: Problems and Possibilities of Narrative. Lincoln: U of Nebraska P.2002) and in the examination of knowledge of intertextual links (Eco Eco, Umberto (1984). The Role of the Reader: Explorations in the Semiotics of Texts. Bloomington: Indiana UP.1984; Genette Genette, Gérard ([1982] 1997). Palimpsests: Literature in the Second Degree. Lincoln: U of Nebraska P.1982).
[10]
Schema theory has also been used to construct new theories about the nature of narrative. Fludernik (Fludernik, Monika (1996). Towards a ‘Natural’ Narratology. London: Routledge.1996) employs it to redefine narrativity (→ Narrativity), suggesting that cognitive parameters which are “constitutive of prototypical human experience” (12) are the main criteria for what makes a story a story, not action sequences as traditionally thought. In her model, “there can therefore be narratives without plot, but there cannot be any narratives without a human (anthropomorphic) experiencer” (13). Herman (Herman, David (2002). Story Logic: Problems and Possibilities of Narrative. Lincoln: U of Nebraska P.2002: 85–6) defines “narrativehood,” his term for the difference between narratives and non-narratives, using scripts. As scripts represent only stereotypical and expected information, the gaps in a text which a script can supply are not unique and hence do not produce narratives in their own right. By contrast, where a gap cannot be filled by stereotypical information, it “focus[es] attention on the unusual and the remarkable” (90) and requires a narrative explanation. For Herman, narrativehood is a binary distinction in contrast to the scalar nature of narrativity, the property of being more or less prototypically a narrative. He argues (91) that maximal narrativity is achieved by balancing the appropriate amount of “canonicity and breach,” using Bruner’s (Bruner, Jerome (1991). “The Narrative Construction of Reality.” Critical Inquiry 18, 1–21.1991) terms. If the majority of events in a story are too stereotypical, they will be untellable and/or uninteresting, but if events are too unusual, the text may not readily be interpreted as a story. Hühn & Kiefer (Hühn, Peter & Jens Kiefer (2005). The Narratological Analysis of Lyric Poetry: Studies in English Poetry from the 16th to the 20th Century. Berlin: de Gruyter.2005) use the term “eventfulness” for deviations from scripts, viewing these deviations as both unexpected events and instances when an expected event does not occur (→ Event and Eventfulness). For them, deviations must be judged by viewing sequences in the context of cultural and historical factors, using schemata to assess the degree of deviation.
[11]
Another important theoretical contribution of schema theory lies in discussions of literariness. Cook (Cook, Guy (1994). Discourse and Literature: The Interplay of Form and Mind. Oxford: Oxford UP.1994) has defined “literariness” as “discourse deviation,” stating that a narrative acquires literary status when it “bring[s] about a change in the schemata of a reader” (182). Cook sees literary discourse as “schema refreshing,” meaning that old schemata may be destroyed, new ones constructed and that new connections may be made between existing schemata (191), in contrast to “schema preserving” or “schema reinforcing” forms of discourse. His theory echoes the Russian formalist idea of defamiliarization as an essential aspect of literary writing and comprehending. Cook’s definition is controversial because texts which are not literary may nevertheless disrupt existing schemata, as Cook himself admits (47, 192) in relation to journalism, science writing and conversation. In addition, Semino (Semino, Elena (1997). Language and World Creation in Poems and Other Texts. London: Longman.1997: 175) argues that literary texts can both challenge and confirm existing beliefs, suggesting a scale of schema refreshment for those which are challenging. This does, however, depend on the historical period: during medieval times, confirmation seems to have dominated, whereas in modern times deviation is generally more prominent (see Lotman’s [(Lotman, Jurij ([1970] 1977). The Structure of the Artistic Text. Ann Arbor: U of Michigan P.1970) 1977: 288–96] concepts of “aesthetics of identity and opposition”). Jeffries (Jeffries, Lesley (2001). “Schema Theory and White Asparagus: Cultural Multilingualism among Readers of Texts.” Language and Literature 10, 325–43.2001), though, highlights the extent to which particular sub-cultures nowadays may still delight in “schema affirmation,” her term for a reader’s “thrill of recognition” of familiar experience in literary texts. A different perspective on the role of schemata is provided by Miall (Miall, David S. (1989). “Beyond the Schema Given: Affective Comprehension of Literary Narratives.” Cognition and Emotion 3, 55–78.1989), who argues that it is a reader’s emotions that primarily help the reader make sense of a defamiliarizing literary text, suggesting that affect is primary in reading and that emotions drive the construction of new schemata rather than being an after-effect of cognitive processing.
[12]
One major use of schema theory has been in the description of “mind style” (Fowler Fowler, Roger ([1986] 1996). Linguistic Criticism. Oxford: Oxford UP.1986) by stylisticians, who use linguistic analysis to study the thought representations of characters who have difficulty comprehending the world around them, such as primitive humans, the mentally impaired, and those alien to a culture (see Semino Semino, Elena (2006). “Mind Style.” K. Brown (ed). Encyclopedia of Language and Linguistics, 2nd edition. Oxford: Elsevier, vol. 8, 142–8.2006 for a summary). Often the technique used by writers is to under-specify (Emmott Emmott, Catherine (2006). “Reference: Stylistic Aspects.” K. Brown (ed). Encyclopedia of Language and Linguistics, 2nd edition. Oxford: Elsevier, vol. 10, 441–50.2006) the references to key aspects of the focalizing character’s context so that the character’s lack of understanding is conveyed, but nevertheless writers still need to give readers enough clues to construe the situation by using familiar schemata. Palmer (Palmer, Alan (2004). Fictional Minds. Lincoln: U of Nebraska P.2004) goes beyond the focus on special types of mind style by suggesting that all fictional minds need to be cognitively constructed by means of “continuing-consciousness frames” in order to bring together diverse mentions of the thoughts of individual characters and groups of characters throughout a story.
[13]
In addition to the above theoretical and descriptive uses, the notion of schemata has an extremely wide range of applications in narrative studies. In feminist stylistics, Mills (Mills, Sara (1995). Feminist Stylistics. London: Routledge.1995: 187–94), has used it to challenge the sexist schemata that she claims are needed to read some literary texts written by men. In humor studies, oddly incongruous frames are often regarded as the source of humor (e.g. Semino Semino, Elena (1997). Language and World Creation in Poems and Other Texts. London: Longman.1997; Hidalgo-Downing Hidalgo-Downing, Laura (2000). Negation, Text Worlds, and Discourse: The Pragmatics of Fiction. Stamford: Ablex.2000; Simpson Simpson, Paul (2003). On the Discourse of Satire: Towards a Stylistic Model of Satirical Humour. Amsterdam: Benjamins.2003). In detective and mystery stories, clues can be buried by making descriptions heavily schema consistent, then subsequently highlighted by adding information over and above the schema (Alexander Alexander, Marc (2006). Cognitive-Linguistic Manipulation and Persuasion in Agatha Christie, M.Phil. thesis. Glasgow: U of Glasgow.2006; Emmott et al. Emmott, Catherine, Anthony J. Sanford & Marc Alexander (2010). “Scenarios, Characters’ Roles and Plot Status. Readers’ Assumtions and Writers’ Manipulations of Assumtions in Narrative Texts”. J. Eder, F. Jannidis & R. Schneider (eds). Characters in Fictional Worlds. Understanding Imaginary Beings in Literature, Film, and Other Media. Berlin: de Gruyter, 377–99.2010). In the analysis of science fiction (Stockwell Stockwell, Peter (2003). “Schema Poetics and Speculative Cosmology”. Language and Literature 12.3, 252–71.2003) and absurdist texts (Semino Semino, Elena (1997). Language and World Creation in Poems and Other Texts. London: Longman.1997; Hidalgo-Downing Hidalgo-Downing, Laura (2000). Negation, Text Worlds, and Discourse: The Pragmatics of Fiction. Stamford: Ablex.2000), schema theory can explain how alternative and bizarre worlds are created. In educational psychology, schemata and scripts explain how children develop their storytelling and comprehension skills (e.g. McCabe & Peterson eds. McCabe, Allyssa & Carole Peterson, eds. (1991). Developing Narrative Structure. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.1991). In film studies (→ Narration in Film), schema theory has been used in discussions of text coherence, genre, and character construction (Bordwell Bordwell, David (1989). Making Meaning: Inference and Rhetoric in the Interpretation of Cinema. Cambridge: Harvard UP.1989: 129–95; Branigan Branigan, David (1992). Narrative Comprehension and Film. London: Routledge.1992: 1–32). This list is not intended to be comprehensive, but gives an indication of the importance of schema theory across a number of areas.
[14]
In recent years, the emphasis within the cognitive study of narrative has shifted somewhat (→ Cognitive Narratology). Schema theory is still viewed as important, but there has been a growing interest in how a reader needs to supplement general knowledge with the knowledge accumulated from the text itself. So readers will normally gather together a large store of information about characters and contexts as they read a text. Emmott (Emmott, Catherine (1997). Narrative Comprehension: A Discourse Perspective. Oxford: Oxford UP.1997) calls this “text-specific knowledge” and argues that readers must not only build mental representations (termed “contextual frames”) using this knowledge, but update these representations where necessary and utilize the information at later stages in a text. Similar ideas can be found in Gerrig’s (Gerrig, Richard J. (1993). Experiencing Narrative Worlds: On the Psychological Activities of Reading. New Haven: Yale UP.1993) examination of narrative worlds, Werth’s (Werth, Paul (1999). Text Worlds: Representing Conceptual Space in Discourse. London: Longman.1999) text world theory, and Herman’s (Herman, David (2002). Story Logic: Problems and Possibilities of Narrative. Lincoln: U of Nebraska P.2002) study of storyworlds.
[15] 4 Topics for Further Investigation
[16]
(a) The inter-relation between schema knowledge and other knowledge (e.g. expert, autobiographical, and text world knowledge) needs to be explored further and built into an overall model with empirical testing of texts which are more complex than traditional psychological and Artificial Intelligence materials. (b) More psychological research is needed to establish how generic knowledge derived from the real world is utilized in building counterfactual worlds, since the findings from current empirical work are not consistent (Nieuwland & van Berkum Nieuwland, Mante S. & Jos J. A. van Berkum (2006). “When Peanuts Fall in Love: N400 Evidence for the Power of Discourse.” Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 19, 1098–111.2006; Ferguson & Sanford Ferguson, Heather J. & Anthony J. Sanford (2008). “Anomalies in Real and Counterfactual Worlds: An Eye-Movement Investigation.” Journal of Memory and Language 58, 609–26.2008). (c) There needs to be additional investigation of how readers use schemata similarly or differently in reading factual and fictional texts. (d) Frames based on “intertextual knowledge” (Eco Eco, Umberto (1984). The Role of the Reader: Explorations in the Semiotics of Texts. Bloomington: Indiana UP.1984; Genette Genette, Gérard ([1982] 1997). Palimpsests: Literature in the Second Degree. Lincoln: U of Nebraska P.1982) need further empirical study.
[17] 5 Bibliography
[18] 5.1 Works Cited
- Alexander, Marc (2006). Cognitive-Linguistic Manipulation and Persuasion in Agatha Christie, M.Phil. thesis. Glasgow: U of Glasgow.
- Bartlett, Frederick C. (1932). Remembering: A Study in Experimental and Social Psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge UP.
- Black, John B. & Robert Wilensky (1979). “An Evaluation of Story Grammars.” Cognitive Science 3, 213–30.
- Bordwell, David (1989). Making Meaning: Inference and Rhetoric in the Interpretation of Cinema. Cambridge: Harvard UP.
- Bower, Gordon, John B. Black & Terrence J. Turner (1979). “Scripts in Memory for Text.” Cognitive Psychology 11, 177–220.
- Branigan, David (1992). Narrative Comprehension and Film. London: Routledge.
- Bruner, Jerome (1991). “The Narrative Construction of Reality.” Critical Inquiry 18, 1–21.
- Cook, Guy (1994). Discourse and Literature: The Interplay of Form and Mind. Oxford: Oxford UP.
- de Beaugrande, Robert (1987). “Schemas for Literary Communication.” L. Halász (ed). Literary Discourse: Aspects of Cognitive and Social Psychological Approaches. Berlin: de Gruyter, 49–99.
- Eco, Umberto (1984). The Role of the Reader: Explorations in the Semiotics of Texts. Bloomington: Indiana UP.
- Emmott, Catherine (1997). Narrative Comprehension: A Discourse Perspective. Oxford: Oxford UP.
- Emmott, Catherine (2006). “Reference: Stylistic Aspects.” K. Brown (ed). Encyclopedia of Language and Linguistics, 2nd edition. Oxford: Elsevier, vol. 10, 441–50.
- Emmott, Catherine, Anthony J. Sanford & Marc Alexander (2010). “Scenarios, Characters’ Roles and Plot Status. Readers’ Assumtions and Writers’ Manipulations of Assumtions in Narrative Texts.” J. Eder, F. Jannidis & R. Schneider (eds). Characters in Fictional Worlds. Understanding Imaginary Beings in Literature, Film, and Other Media. Berlin: de Gruyter, 377–99.
- Ferguson, Heather J. & Anthony J. Sanford (2008). “Anomalies in Real and Counterfactual Worlds: An Eye-Movement Investigation.” Journal of Memory and Language 58, 609–26.
- Fludernik, Monika (1996). Towards a ‘Natural’ Narratology. London: Routledge.
- Fowler, Roger ([1986] 1996). Linguistic Criticism. Oxford: Oxford UP.
- Genette, Gérard ([1982] 1997). Palimpsests: Literature in the Second Degree. Lincoln: U of Nebraska P.
- Gerrig, Richard J. (1993). Experiencing Narrative Worlds: On the Psychological Activities of Reading. New Haven: Yale UP.
- Herman, David (2002). Story Logic: Problems and Possibilities of Narrative. Lincoln: U of Nebraska P.
- Hidalgo-Downing, Laura (2000). Negation, Text Worlds, and Discourse: The Pragmatics of Fiction. Stamford: Ablex.
- Hühn, Peter & Jens Kiefer (2005). The Narratological Analysis of Lyric Poetry: Studies in English Poetry from the 16th to the 20th Century. Berlin: de Gruyter.
- Ingarden, Roman ([1931] 1973). The Literary Work of Art: An Investigation on the Borderlines of Ontology, Logic and Theory of Literature. Evanston: Northwestern UP.
- Iser, Wolfgang ([1976] 1978). The Act of Reading: A Theory of Aesthetic Response. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
- Jeffries, Lesley (2001). “Schema Theory and White Asparagus: Cultural Multilingualism among Readers of Texts.” Language and Literature 10, 325–43.
- Johnson-Laird, Philip (1983). Mental Models. Cambridge: Cambridge UP.
- Koffka, Kurt (1935). Principles of Gestalt Psychology. London: Kegan Paul, Trench, Trubner.
- Köhler, Wolfgang (1930). Gestalt Psychology. London: Bell.
- Lotman, Jurij ([1970] 1977). The Structure of the Artistic Text. Ann Arbor: U of Michigan P.
- Mandler, Jean M. (1984). Scripts, Stories and Scenes: Aspects of Schema Theory. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.
- Mandler, Jean M. & Nancy S. Johnson (1977). “Remembrance of Things Parsed: Story Structure and Recall.” Cognitive Psychology 9, 111–51.
- McCabe, Allyssa & Carole Peterson, eds. (1991). Developing Narrative Structure. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.
- Miall, David S. (1989). “Beyond the Schema Given: Affective Comprehension of Literary Narratives.” Cognition and Emotion 3, 55–78.
- Mills, Sara (1995). Feminist Stylistics. London: Routledge.
- Minsky, Marvin (1975). “A Framework for Representing Knowledge.” P. H. Winston (ed). The Psychology of Computer Vision. New York: McGraw-Hill, 211–77.
- Nieuwland, Mante S. & Jos J. A. van Berkum (2006). “When Peanuts Fall in Love: N400 Evidence for the Power of Discourse.” Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 19, 1098–111.
- Palmer, Alan (2004). Fictional Minds. Lincoln: U of Nebraska P.
- Rumelhart, David E. (1975). “Notes on a Schema for Stories.” D. G. Bobrow & A. Collins (eds). Representation and Understanding: Studies in Cognitive Science. New York: Academic P, 211–35.
- Rumelhart, David E. (1980). “Schemata: The Building Blocks of Cognition.” R. Spiro, B. Bruce & W. Brewer (eds). Theoretical Issues in Reading Comprehension. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum, 33–58.
- Rumelhart, David E. & Donald A. Norman (1978). “Accretion, Tuning and Restructuring: Three Modes of Learning.” J. W. Cotton & R. Klatzky (eds). Semantic Factors in Cognition. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum, 37–53.
- Sanford, Anthony J. & Simon C. Garrod (1981). Understanding Written Language: Explorations in Comprehension Beyond the Sentence. Chichester: Wiley.
- Schank, Roger C. (1982). Dynamic Memory: A Theory of Reminding and Learning. Cambridge: Cambridge UP.
- Schank, Roger C. & Robert P. Abelson (1977). Scripts, Plans, Goals and Understanding. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.
- Semino, Elena (1997). Language and World Creation in Poems and Other Texts. London: Longman.
- Semino, Elena (2006). “Mind Style.” K. Brown (ed). Encyclopedia of Language and Linguistics, 2nd edition. Oxford: Elsevier, vol. 8, 142–8.
- Simpson, Paul (2003). On the Discourse of Satire: Towards a Stylistic Model of Satirical Humour. Amsterdam: Benjamins.
- Sternberg, Meir (1978). Expositional Modes and Temporal Ordering in Fiction. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins UP.
- Sternberg, Meir (1985). The Poetics of Biblical Narrative: Ideological Literature and the Drama of Reading. Bloomington: Indiana UP.
- Stockwell, Peter (2003). “Schema Poetics and Speculative Cosmology.” Language and Literature 12.3, 252–71.
- Werth, Paul (1999). Text Worlds: Representing Conceptual Space in Discourse. London: Longman.
- Wertheimer, Max ([1923] 1938). “Laws of Organization in Perceptual Forms.” W. D. Ellis (ed). A Source Book of Gestalt Psychology. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul, 71–88.
- Wertheimer, Max ([1925] 1938). “Gestalt Theory.” W. D. Ellis (ed). A Source Book of Gestalt Psychology. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul, 1–11.
[19] 5.2 Further Reading
- Ryan, Marie-Laure (1991). Possible Worlds, Artificial Intelligence, and Narrative Theory. Bloomington: Indiana UP.
- Semino, Elena (2001). “On Readings, Literariness and Schema Theory: A Reply to Jeffries.” Language and Literature 10, 345–55.
- Stockwell, Peter (2006). “Schema Theory: Stylistic Applications.” K. Brown (ed). Encyclopedia of Language and Linguistics, 2nd edition. Oxford: Elsevier, vol. 11, 8–73.
[20]
Acknowledgement
[21]
The authors are grateful to the Arts and Humanities Research Council for funding for this work, which was conducted as part of the STACS Project (Stylistics, Text Analysis and Cognitive Science: Interdisciplinary Perspectives on the Nature of Reading).
Contents |
New address
Since May 1, 2013, the living handbook of narratology (LHN) appears as a CMS-based version under the new address:
http://www.lhn.uni-hamburg.de
The former wiki version remains preserved under the date April, 30, 2013. It is now archived as a static version [February, 05, 2019].